The Science of Reading. Группа авторов
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Science of Reading - Группа авторов страница 27

Название: The Science of Reading

Автор: Группа авторов

Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited

Жанр: Языкознание

Серия:

isbn: 9781119705130

isbn:

СКАЧАТЬ M. I., Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, 240, 1627–1631. doi: 10.1126/science.3289116.

      120 Pringle‐Morgan, W. (1896). A case of congenital word blindness. British Medical Journal, 2, 1378. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.1871.1378.

      121 Pritchard, S. C., Coltheart, M., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2016). Modelling the implicit learning of phonological decoding from training on whole‐word spellings and pronunciations. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(1), 49–63. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2015.1085384.

      122 Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 29–53. doi: 10.2307/747832.

      123 Ramus. F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/s0959‐4388(03)00035‐7.

      124 Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Masked phonological priming effects in English: Are they real? Do they matter? Cognitive Psychology, 53(2), 97–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.01.002.

      125 Rayner, K., Ashby, J., Pollatsek, A., & Reichle, E. D. (2004). The effects of frequency and predictability on eye fixations in reading: Implications for the EZ reader model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(4), 720–732. doi: 10.1037/0096‐1523.30.4.720.

      126 Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M.S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2(2), 31–74. A supplement to Psychological Science. doi: 10.1111/1529‐1006.00004.

      127 Rayner, K., Juhasz, B. J. & Pollatsek, A. (2005). Eye movements during reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227–247). Oxford: Blackwell.

      128 Reicher, G. M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(2), 275–280. doi: 10.1037/h0027768.

      129 Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125–157. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.105.1.125.

      130 Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E‐Z Reader model of eye‐movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 445–476. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x03000104.

      131 Rueckl, J. G., Paz‐Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W.‐J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., … Frost, R. (2015). Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences, 112, 15510–15515. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509321112.

      132 Saygin, Z. M., Osher, D. E., Norton, E. S., Youssoufian, D. A., Beach, S. D., Feather, J., … Kanwisher, N. (2016). Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nature Neuroscience, 19(9), 1250–1255. doi: 10.1038/nn.4354.

      133 Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goal, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi: 10.4324/9780203781036.

      134 Seidenberg, M. S. (2011). Reading in different writing systems: One architecture, multiple solutions. In P. McCardle, J. Ren, O. Tzeng, & B. Miller (Eds.), Dyslexia across languages: Orthography and the brain‐gene‐behavior link (pp. 146–168). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

      135 Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96(4), 523–568. doi: 10.1037/0033‐295x.96.4.523.

      136 Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self‐teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010‐0277(94)00645‐2.

      137 Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self‐teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(4), 267–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.001.

      138 Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615. doi: 10.1037/0033‐2909.134.4.584.

      139 Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E. Blachman, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., … Gore, J. C., (2004). Development of left occipito‐temporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically‐based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55(9), 926–933. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.019.

      140 Shu, H., McBride‐Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133. doi: 10.1037/0022‐0663.98.1.122.

      141 Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M. Sarkari, S., Billingsley, R. L, Denton, C., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2007). Altering the brain circuits for reading through intervention: A magnetic source imaging study. Neuropsychology, 21(4), 485–496. doi: 10.1037/0894‐4105.21.4.485.

      142 Singer, M., Halldorson, M., Lear, J. C., & Andrusiak, P. (1992). Validation of causal bridging inferences in discourse understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 507–524. doi: 10.1016/0749‐596X(92)90026‐T.

      143 Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading constrained by culture: Evidence from Chinese. Nature, 431, 71–76. doi: 10.1038/nature02865.

      144 Snell, J., van Leipsig, S., Grainger, J., & Meeter, M. (2018). OB1‐reader: A model of word recognition and eye movements in text reading. Psychological Review, 125(6), 969–984. doi: 10.1037/rev0000119.

      145 Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373. doi: 10.1111/1467‐8624.7402003.

      146 Snowling, M. J., Lervåg, A., Nash, H. M., & Hulme, C. (2019). Longitudinal relationships between speech perception, phonological skills and reading in children at high‐risk of dyslexia. Developmental science, 22(1), e12723. doi: 10.1111/desc.12723.

      147 Snowling, M. J., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. P. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: A developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309–339. doi: 10.1080/02643298608253362.

      148 Stafura, J. Z., & Perfetti, C. A. (2014). Word‐to‐text integration: Message level and lexical level influences in ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 64, 41–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.012.

      149 Stein, N., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse processing: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

      150 Strong, G. K., Torgerson, C. J., Torgerson, D., & Hulme, C. (2011). A systematic meta‐analytic review of evidence for the effectiveness of the “Fast ForWord” language intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 224–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‐7610.2010.02329.x.

      151 Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9(2), 182–198. doi: 10.1016/0093‐934x(80)90139‐x.

      152 Tan, L.H., Spinks, J.A., Eden, G., Perfetti, C.A., & Siok, W.T. (2005). СКАЧАТЬ