Introducing Second Language Acquisition. Kirsten M. Hummel
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Introducing Second Language Acquisition - Kirsten M. Hummel страница 12

Название: Introducing Second Language Acquisition

Автор: Kirsten M. Hummel

Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited

Жанр: Языкознание

Серия:

isbn: 9781119554295

isbn:

СКАЧАТЬ less broadly than its true domain of reference, also occurs, although it is more difficult to detect, since it is not as obvious if a child is failing to provide a label as when actually mislabeling as in overextension. An example of underextension is a child using the word “dog” only for his or her pet collie, but not for the neighbor's pet poodle. Various estimates suggest as many as 30% of words are overextended at least some of the time during the first two years of the child (e.g. Clark 1993; Rescorla 1980). By 2.5 years of age, however, only rare occasions of over‐ or underextensions are thought to occur.

       overextension

      A child's use of a word for objects or items that share a feature or property; for example, using “dog” to refer to all animals with four legs.

       underextension

      A child's use of a word with a narrower meaning than in the adult's language; for example, “dog” to refer only to the family's pet.

      Some children have been observed to go through a “word spurt” period that has been found to begin when they are about 18 months old and which lasts for a few months (e.g. Clark 1993). During this period, new words spring up in the child's vocabulary on an almost daily basis. Some researchers (Goldfield and Reznick 1990) suggest only some children show a spurt while others show a linear pattern of vocabulary growth. One proposed explanation for the word spurt is “fast mapping,” i.e. that children are able to remember a word after very limited exposure to that word.

      Another important stage occurs when children begin to link together more than two words, and enter what has been termed the “telegraphic stage.” At this point, children may produce strings of two‐, three‐, and even four‐word long units, sometimes more. The label “telegraphic” is used to reflect the fact that these strings tend to omit function words, such as articles, conjunctions, and prepositions, and largely consist of content words, such as nouns and verbs. For example, one child beginning to grow beyond the two‐word stage was heard to say “Baby Allison comb hair.”

       morphemes

      Smallest meaning‐bearing unit of language (e.g. word units, like “dog,” and grammatical inflections, like the plural “–s”).

       mean length of utterance (MLU)

      Measurement used to calculate the development of children's grammar; number of morphemes divided by number of total utterances.

image

      Source: Berko Gleason, J. and Bernstein Ratner, N. (2017).

      Source: Based on Brown, R. (1973).

–ing
in, on
plural –s
possessive –s
the, a
past tense –ed
3rd person singular –s
auxiliary “be”

       input

      The language to which an individual is exposed in the environment.

image

      Source: Berko, J. (1958). © 1958, Taylor & Francis.