Название: Bats of Southern and Central Africa
Автор: Ara Monadjem
Издательство: Ingram
Жанр: Биология
isbn: 9781776145843
isbn:
Figure 35. Rhinolophus capensis in pursuit of a moth. The dashed lines depict the echoes perceived by the hunting bat.
Not all vocalisations produced by bats are echolocation calls. For example, the audible squeaks bats make in their roosts, the calls that mother and young make to one another, or the calls flying bats make to defend their foraging territories, are not echolocation calls. Instead, these are usually referred to as social calls and are less well understood than echolocation calls.
The echolocation frequencies of most bat species are ultrasonic (i.e. above the range of human hearing), and peak echolocation frequencies (i.e. the frequencies with the highest intensity) usually fall within 20–60 kHz (Fenton 1990). This may be due to the frequency-dependent effects of atmospheric attenuation and target strength (Jones and Rydell 2004). In contrast, many social calls are audible to humans.
Echolocation coupled with flight enables bats to capture nocturnal flying insects in a variety of habitats. This ability probably explains how the radiation of bat species has manifested into the highest trophic diversity among mammals (Patterson et al. 2004, Roemer et al. 2019). Nevertheless, not all bats echolocate (e.g. fruit bats from the family Pteropodidae, except Rousettus species), nor do all echolocating bats use the same type of echolocation.
ECHOLOCATION SYSTEMS
Two different echolocation systems – high and low duty-cycle echolocation – evolved independently in the Chiroptera (Eick et al. 2005). Low duty-cycle echolocation bats emit narrowband or broadband sound pulses separated by inter-pulse intervals that are much longer than the duration of the emitted pulses. Such species therefore separate the emitted pulse from the returning echo in time (Fenton 1990).
High duty-cycle bats emit long, narrowband pulses that are separated by much shorter inter-pulse intervals. Consequently, the emitted signals often overlap with the returning echo. However, the overlap does not produce masking effects (Schnitzler and Kalko 2001), because Doppler-shift compensation keeps the target echo in the range of the neurons of the acoustic fovea – a group of neurons sharply tuned to a very narrow frequency band, a few kHz higher than the peak frequency of the emitted signal (Neuweiler 1990). The Doppler effect is the change in frequency of a sound wave as perceived by a listener moving relative to the source of the sound. For example, the frequency of the siren of a passing emergency vehicle will start out high, slide down as it passes by the listener, and continue lower as it recedes into the distance. In other words, high duty-cycle bats separate the emitted pulse from the returning echo in frequency rather than time (Fenton 1990).
TYPES OF BAT ECHOLOCATION PULSES
Broadband, low duty-cycle, frequency-modulated (LD-FM) echolocation pulses typically sweep downward through as much as an octave for a short duration of time (Fenton 1990, Schnitzler and Kalko 2001) (Figure 36). At the same time, the bat senses with increasing precision the range and position of the object in space – its localisation (Simmons and Stein 1980). This is because LD-FM signals sweep rapidly through the corresponding neural filters, and are therefore reliable time markers to determine the range of the target from the foraging bat (Moss and Schnitzler 1995). In addition, the neuronal filters are activated across a broad frequency range, which increases the reliability of the monaural and binaural cues the bat uses to localise the target in space (Schnitzler and Kalko 2001). Nevertheless, LD-FM signals are less suited for the detection of distant and/or weak echoes, because the neuronal filters are activated for only a short time (Schnitzler and Kalko 2001).
Figure 36. Diagram showing sonograms of southern African bats that use high duty-cycle and low duty-cycle echolocation calls.
Narrowband, low duty-cycle pulses composed of constant frequency (LD-CF) or shallow frequency-modulated (LD-QCF) components (Figure 36) are not suitable for localisation of a hunted target, but are well suited to detection, because they activate the neuronal filters of the corresponding narrow frequency band during the entire echo (Schnitzler and Kalko 2001). In addition, acoustic ‘glints’ (short prominent amplitude peaks in the echo that are created when a fluttering insect’s wing is perpendicular to the incoming sound wave) can be 20–30 decibel (dB) stronger than the echo from the body of the insect; it is these glints off the flying insect’s wings that further increase the likelihood of its detection by the bat (Kober and Schnitzler 1980, Moss and Zagaeski 1994).
In contrast to low duty-cycle bats, Doppler-shift compensation combined with a specialised auditory system enables constant frequency high duty-cycle (HD-CF) echolocating bats to localise and classify fluttering insects in dense (clutter) habitats (Schnitzler and Kalko 2001). HD-CF bats can classify insects by listening to the unique acoustic glints imprinted by the fluttering wings of different insects onto the echoes of their CF calls (Schnitzler 1987, von der Emde and Menne 1989, von der Emde and Schnitzler 1990) (Figure 36).
ECOLOGY OF BAT ECHOLOCATION AND FLIGHT
Bat species exhibit great diversity in how and where they fly. The most important ecological constraint on foraging insectivorous bats is clutter, that is, the number of obstacles a bat has to detect and avoid (Fenton 1990). Vegetation structure has an overriding control over the relative clutter of the habitats exploited by foraging bats. Perceptually, bats are constrained by the capabilities of their sensory mechanisms (e.g. echolocation, vision, olfaction, hearing) to detect, classify, and localise potential prey near clutter. Echolocation is ineffective over long ranges, and therefore sets spatial limits on where bats can forage. Geometric and atmospheric attenuation severely reduce the intensity of echolocation echoes with increasing target distance (Pye 1980, Lawrence and Simmons 1982). Furthermore, the intensity of the target echo depends on the size and shape of the target (Barclay and Brigham 1991, Waters et al. 1995). Mechanically, bats are constrained by the capabilities of their flying ability (Figure 37). These factors explain why the flight morphology and echolocation systems of bats are adapted specifically to the habitat structure in which they forage (Aldridge and Rautenbach 1987, Norberg and Rayner 1987, Roemer et al. 2019). For example, a wing shape that allows fast flight would be useless if coupled with echolocation calls that are effective only at short range. Such a bat would not be able to detect prey soon enough to capture it. Similarly, wing morphology adapted for slow, manoeuvrable flight in clutter needs to be coupled with echolocation signals that can distinguish prey from clutter echoes. Wing morphology and echolocation signals are therefore part of the same adaptive complex (Arita and Fenton 1997). Based on these adaptations, sympatric bats can be classified into three broad functional foraging groups: open, clutter-edge, and clutter foragers (Crome and Richards 1988, Fullard et al. 1991, Schnitzler and Kalko 2001, Schoeman and Jacobs 2008).
Figure 37. Adaptations of wing shape and the resulting flight style to different foraging habitats. Fast fliers (e.g. Tadarida) hunt above the vegetation (open-air functional foraging group). These bats have narrow, long wings, wide wingspans, and high wing loading. Species that are highly manoeuvrable (e.g. Hipposideros) forage in and near vegetation (clutter functional СКАЧАТЬ