The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов страница 60

СКАЧАТЬ 197–212.

      51 Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(1), 139–184.

      52 Smith, P. K. (2005). Play: types and functions in human development. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 271–291). Guilford.

      53 Taylor, S., & Workman, L. (2018). The psychology of human social development. Routledge.

      54 Tomasello, M. (2009). Why we cooperate. MIT Press.

      55 Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press.

      56 Vandell, D. L. (2000). Parents, peer groups, and other socializing influences. Developmental Psychology, 36(6), 600–710.

      57 Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, 22(2), 267–273.

      58 West‐Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.

      59 Workman, L. (2014). Charles Darwin: Shaper of evolutionary thinking. Palgrave Macmillan.

      60 Workman, L., Adam, J., & Andrew, R. J. (2000). Opportunities for visual experience which might allow imprinting in chicks raised by broody hens. Behaviour, 137(2), 221–231.

      61 Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2021). Evolutionary psychology (4th ed.). Cambridge University Press.

       Willem Koops

      To understand and develop ideas on childhood, child development, and education, empirical studies of children’s behavior cannot suffice. Apart from “normal” empirical analytical knowledge, or rather datasets, it is very important to study the cultural‐historical context of this “normal science” and its predecessors (Koops & Kessel, 2017; Mintz, 2017).

      In this chapter we first revisit the philosophical foundation of developmental thinking and its application to understanding children. This brings the reader to the great German philosopher Kant, and to the founder of child development and education, Rousseau. In particular, Rousseau’s book on the boy Émile will be presented as the core publication of modern thoughts on childhood and education, followed by the reception of this book by historical founders of developmental psychology and pedagogy in Germany, Switzerland, and The Netherlands.

      After the explanation of the role of Enlightenment thinking in terms of “progress” and development, it will be made clear that one essential feature of the Enlightenment philosophy is disputable: the unjustified belief in “progress.” This idea of “progress” is fully present in the original developmental ideas of Rousseau, and also of his later follower Piaget. It will be explained in this chapter that neo‐ and post‐neo‐Piagetian research essentially leads us back to the insight that children are serious interlocutors at a much younger age than we have been inclined to think since the Rousseau‐Piaget tradition (Koops, 2016).

      Finally, it will be concluded that the study of the cultural history of childhood and child development makes clear that child development is less (biologically) continuous than has been the assumption since Rousseau. Post‐neo‐Piagetian research (for example about the Child’s Theory of Mind), makes clear that children are much more serious, if not adult, interlocutors. We may speak of a historical reframing of childhood, with numerous new possibilities for pedagogy and education.

Photo depicts the statue of Immanuel Kant (1724–1804). Photo depicts Jean Jacques Rousseau (1712–1778).

      Source: Allan Ramsay, Jean‐Jacques Rousseau, National Gallery of Scotland).