Название: The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development
Автор: Группа авторов
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Жанр: Общая психология
isbn: 9781119678991
isbn:
Research on prenatal and infant development strengthened the premise that neurological development underlies early‐emerging social abilities and, when compromised, contributes to social dysfunctions. Findings, for example, implied that infants are biologically prepared to attend and respond to caregivers and to participate in rudimentary forms of social interaction (Bornstein, 2013). In contrast, neurological impairments attributable to prenatal teratogens (e.g., cocaine, marijuana, alcohol) were linked with early‐emerging, and in some cases, lasting social and emotional difficulties (Behnke et al., 2013).
Studies of brain maturation revealed that, throughout childhood and adolescence, there were periods of rapid growth and pruning within specific brain regions (e.g., visual, motor, auditory, language centers) and progress toward hemispheric lateralization and connectedness. Growth of this type was found to be associated with improvements in infants’ and children’s social and emotional capacities, skills, and regulatory abilities (Nelson et al., 2006).
Neuroscientific findings also supported the conclusion that social experience is a necessary component of brain development. Evidence implied that, although the brain is wired to “expect” species‐wide forms of experience (i.e., experience‐expectant processes), it also is influenced by experiences that are unique to the individual (i.e., experience‐dependent processes; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). On this basis, it was argued that experience essentially “customizes” the child’s brain (e.g., builds, differentially strengthens, prunes neuronal networks). Support for this contention included evidence linking brain alterations with differences in children’s early experiences and rearing conditions (e.g., deprivation, isolation, abuse; Nelson et al., 2007).
Recent innovations include the construct of the “social brain” which was conceived to be a network of brain sites (Blakemore, 2008; Bornstein, 2013) that facilitated the processing and interpretation of social phenomena (e.g., recognizing faces, predicting another’s actions). It was postulated that this network, along with other brain regions, contained a “mirror” system of neurons that enabled children to empathize or “experience” the same emotions they observed in others. Imaging data provided considerable support for the existence of these networks and their hypothesized functions (Blakemore, 2008).
Temperament
Beginning in the 1950s, investigators such as Thomas and Chess (Thomas et al., 1968) utilized the construct of temperament to account for early‐emerging, stable individual differences in infants’ and children’s responses to their environments. Subsequent refinements to theory and research extended knowledge about the origins of temperament. Much of what was learned supported the hypothesis that temperament, although not impervious to environmental influences, has a genetic or neurological basis (Kagan, 2007).
Advances were made in the conceptualization of temperament, the specification of component dimensions, and the development of reliable assessments. Descriptive taxonomies (e.g., easy, difficult, slow‐to‐warm‐up) were replaced with theory‐driven models that attributed temperamental variations to specific emotional, cognitive, or behavioral processes (emotional reactivity to novel stimuli; Kagen & Snidman, 2007; effortful control, negative affectivity, extroversion‐surgency; Rothbart, 2007). Empirical work clarified how temperamental characteristics were related to other aspects of children’s social development, including their behavior, relationships, and adjustment (Rothbart 2007).
Aim 3: Explicate the nature and development of children’s internal social‐cognitive, psychological, and emotional processes
Another prominent objective was to expand knowledge about processes that transpire within the child, that is, nonobservables such as social‐cognitive, psychological, and emotional representations and processes. Largely, investigative efforts were focused on explicating theory (e.g., specifying mechanisms driving development), gathering evidence about developmental transformations (i.e., age or stage changes, growth patterns), and linking specific internal processes with other aspects of child development (e.g., behavior, health, dysfunction).
Self‐understanding
Priorities within this sphere were to further illuminate when children develop a sense of self (i.e., emergence of self‐recognition), how children revise their self‐construals with age and experience (i.e., development of self‐concept), and how children appraise their abilities and worth (i.e., self‐esteem). Findings suggested that self‐recognition emerges early (i.e., around age two) and becomes more reliable and less context‐dependent across early childhood (Miyazaki & Hiraki, 2006).
Constructivist perspectives dominated research on self‐concept and, for the most part, investigators examined stability and change children’s self‐construals using self‐descriptive tools and methodologies. Principal findings implied that children’s self‐theories become more complex and abstract as they are shaped by age‐, gender‐, and context‐related experiences (Harter, 2012).
Self‐esteem was investigated for scientific purposes and to address the popularized assumption that high self‐esteem is a prerequisite for children’s achievement and well‐being. Discoveries illuminated developmental shifts in children’s self‐evaluations, suggesting a progression from global and unrealistic appraisals during early childhood to more domain‐specific (i.e., ability‐based) and accurate appraisals during middle childhood and thereafter. Findings also revealed that children’s overall sense of self‐worth varied as a function of their perceived competence within multiple, specific domains (e.g., social, scholastic, athletic; Harter, 2012).
Potential causes and consequences of self‐esteem were explored by examining dimensions of children’s social relations and experiences. Evidence indicated, for example, that self‐esteem correlated positively with warm, affectionate parent–child relations (Ojanen & Perry, 2007), and negatively with parental abuse (Cicchetti & Toth, 2006). Likewise, children were found to have higher self‐esteem when their peer relationships were supportive as opposed to abusive (e.g., friendships vs. peer victimization; Hodges et al., 1999). Decrements in self‐esteem were documented following school transitions (Wigfield & Eccles, 1994) and parental divorce (Bynum & Durm, 1996). The premise that self‐esteem is a precondition for healthy development received mixed support in that higher levels of this construct were linked with positive as well as negative outcomes (e.g., happiness, lower internalizing problems vs. drug use, prejudice; Baumeister et al., 2003).
Social‐cognition
In addition to the self, investigators explored children’s cognitions about others. Principal foci included research on children’s inferences about others’ mental states and psychological traits (e.g., intentions, personalities), and their ability to use these and other insights to negotiate interactions and solve social problems.
Research on theory of mind helped to clarify when children begin to draw inferences about others’ thoughts and beliefs, and how they utilize these conjectures to forecast their own and others’ social behavior. Evidence suggested that abilities such as these emerged during СКАЧАТЬ