Introduction to Abnormal Child and Adolescent Psychology. Robert Weis
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Introduction to Abnormal Child and Adolescent Psychology - Robert Weis страница 42

СКАЧАТЬ & Flewelling, 1994).

      Unfortunately, later studies were unable to reproduce the initial results. Overall, the students who received D.A.R.E. did not differ in their attitudes toward drugs, involvement with the police, or self-esteem from students who never participated in the program (West & O’Neal, 2004). More concerning, some children who participated in D.A.R.E. were more likely to use alcohol and other drugs during adolescence than children who did not participate in the program (Sloboda et al., 2009).

      Although the early benefits of D.A.R.E. could not be replicated, the program continues to be offered in many school districts across the country. The program has been revised and rebranded. Some studies suggest that this new program may be more effective than the original D.A.R.E., but replication is needed before we can be confident in these findings (Caputi & McLellan, 2018).

      The field of psychology devotes too little time and effort to reproducibility. Psychology journals tend to favor new and exciting research findings over replications. As a result, only about 1% of published research studies are replications (Makel, Plucker, & Hegarty, 2012).

      Some psychologists have tried to make up for this shortcoming in replication research by attempting to reproduce the results of psychological studies published in well-respected journals. The largest attempt was led by Brian Nosek and the Open Science Collaboration (2015), a team of 270 researchers who performed direct replications of 100 studies in the fields of cognitive and social psychology. Unfortunately, the researchers were able to replicate only 36% of these studies. This low replication rate prompted some experts to claim that psychology is experiencing a replication crisis, that is, a failure to be able to reproduce its findings. Low replication rates have also been seen in other fields such as business, economics, and medicine (Martin & Clarke, 2018).

      Recently, clinical psychologists have tried to encourage more replication research (Cybulski, Mayo-Wilson, & Grant, 2017). The Center for Open Science has created a repository where researchers can share data and report their replication studies. Similarly, the journal Perspectives on Psychological Science maintains a record of replication studies. Clinical researchers are also encouraged to preregister their treatment studies. Preregistration involves reporting the study’s purpose, design, and methods prior to observing its results. If the results show a treatment to be ineffective, then other researchers would have access to that important information (Nosek & Lindsay, 2018).

       Review

       Pseudoscience consists of statements, beliefs, and practices that seem to be based on empirical evidence but are actually incompatible with scientific thinking. It capitalizes on our inherent cognitive and emotional biases.

       Science is a set of principles and procedures used to understand the natural world. It relies on careful, systematic collection of empirical data rather than expectations, emotions, or casual observations.

       Science is characterized by (1) falsifiability, (2) critical thinking, (3) parsimony, (4) precision, and (5) reproducibility.

      What Is the Scientific Method?

      In addition to the five core principles of scientific thinking, psychological scientists also follow the scientific method: procedures used to gather, evaluate, and organize information about the natural world (Figure 3.1).

      At the heart of the scientific method is a theory. A psychological theory is an integrated set of ideas that explain and predict broad aspects of human behavior or development. For example, Jean Piaget (1964) developed a theory of cognitive development in which children progress through four discrete stages from infancy to adulthood. Piaget’s theory organizes our understanding of cognitive abilities across the lifespan and allows us to make predictions about the problem-solving skills of children at various ages.

      Similarly, Albert Bandura (1978) developed social learning theory. He believed that children model the behavior of other people, especially when those people are reinforced for acting a certain way. Bandura’s theory organizes our knowledge about the way children learn information and acquire new skills. It lets us predict how children might act when exposed to certain types of models, such as violent video game characters or prosocial peers.

      Theories are usually too broad and abstract to test in a single research study. So, scientists develop hypotheses about one or more parts of the theory. A hypothesis is a specific, falsifiable prediction about the relationship between two or more variables. For example, a researcher might hypothesize that children who play violent video games will be more likely to show aggression than children who do not play violent video games. The researcher’s hypothesis tests one aspect of social learning theory, namely, that exposure to violent games will increase children’s likelihood of aggression.

A process diagram shows the four components of the scientific process: Theory, hypothesis, data collection, and replicate or revise connected as a cycle by four arrows. An illustration of a woman loking at a microscope is in the center of the circle.

      Figure 3.1 ■ The Scientific Method

      Note: Psychologists use the scientific method to gather, evaluate, and organize information about human behavior and development.

      Scientists use empirical data to test hypotheses. Empirical data refer to information gathered through our senses. Psychologists might observe children during class or recess, ask their parents to complete rating scales about children’s behavior at home, or administer tests to assess children’s intelligence or personality. Scientists do not rely on information from nonempirical sources, such as their personal beliefs, feelings, or the opinions of people in authority.

      In order to test a hypothesis using empirical data, researchers must operationally define each variable in the study. An operational definition specifies how each variable will be measured. For example, a researcher might operationally define a child’s “exposure to violent video games” as the number of games the child owns that are rated Mature or Adults Only by the Entertainment Software Rating Board. Similarly, the researcher might operationally define “aggression” as the number of times in the past year the child engaged in pushing, shoving, hitting, or physical fighting at school.

      After collecting data, researchers will analyze and interpret their findings. If the results of the study support the researchers’ hypotheses, then they can have greater confidence in the theory upon which their hypotheses are based. Ideally, the researchers will try to replicate their study to see if their findings are reproducible and can be applied to different people or situations.

      If the results of the study do not support the researchers’ hypotheses, the researchers may need to modify their theory. Even unsuccessful studies are important because they can help to improve theories and build a better understanding of children’s behavior and development.

       Review

       The scientific method is a characteristic set of procedures used to gather, evaluate, and organize empirical data about the natural world.

       Psychological scientists generate and test specific, falsifiable hypotheses based on broader theories regarding behavior and development.

       Scientists rely on empirical data to test hypotheses. They operationally define constructs so they can measure them with greater precision and so other scientists can reproduce their findings.

      3.2 СКАЧАТЬ