Название: Doing Field Projects
Автор: John Forrest
Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited
Жанр: Культурология
isbn: 9781119734628
isbn:
Malinowski’s contribution to anthropological data collecting cannot be underestimated, and the range of his subject matter is astounding. His method is sometimes referred to now as an “off the verandah” technique, meaning that he broke with the custom of interviewing indigenous people on a hotel verandah, and, instead, lived and worked with them in their local villages – learning the local language rather than relying on an interpreter. The methodological point that Malinowski stresses in Argonauts of the Western Pacific, his best-selling, compendious account of island to island exchange (kula ring) and ocean-going canoe travel in Melanesia, is that the participant observer is both intimately involved in the culture under study while, at the same time, scientifically detached. For example, at one point he writes that the goal of the ethnographer in the field is “to grasp the native’s point of view, his relation to life, to realize his vision of his world” (Malinowski 1961 [1922]:25).
Today, anthropologists often use the term “emic” to denote discussion of social situations in local terms (Malinowski’s “native’s point of view”) and “etic” to refer to the anthropologist’s perspective. These terms are derived from Kenneth Pike’s analysis of language. In his Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Structure of Human Behavior (Pike [1954, 1955, 1960] 1967), Pike draws a technical distinction between a “phonetic” and a “phonemic” analysis of the sounds of a language. Phonetic differences are any differences in the sound of a word that can be detected, and phonemic differences are differences that make a difference to the semantic meaning of the word in that language. For example, English speakers can detect the difference between an aspirated and unaspirated /p/ when it is pointed out to them (phonetic difference), but the difference makes no difference to the meanings of words in English (phonemic difference). If you hold your hand close to your mouth and say the word “paper,” you should feel a puff of air on the first /p/ and not on the second. The first /p/ is aspirated, the second is not. You can experiment to make them both aspirated or both unaspirated, but the meaning of the word does not change whichever way you say it. Linguists say that there is a phonetic difference (you can detect the difference), but not a phonemic difference (the meaning of the word does not change). In languages such as Khmer, aspirated and unaspirated /p/ are both phonetically and phonemically different. Using one versus the other changes the meanings of words.
Strip off the /phon/ component of Pike’s terminology (“phone” = “sound”) and you have “etic” and “emic.” You can add new prefixes to get such concepts as “proxemics” – documenting the use of interpersonal space to infer what different distances mean locally (see Chapter 6) – or “aesthemics” (as opposed to “aesthetics”) – understanding value judgments concerning beauty in indigenous terms. Or, you can simply use the suffixes “emic” and “etic” as analytic words in their own right (pronounced /eemic/ (long e) and /etic/ (short e) – same is in “phonemic” and “phonetic”).The emic/etic distinction is of considerable importance in anthropology in general and in fieldwork methodology in particular. It often, simplistically, gets translated into: ways of viewing things from an “insider” (emic) versus “outsider” (etic) perspective in anthropological discourse, but such a translation is rather misleading, although not entirely inaccurate.1 Pike, as a linguist, frequently used the phrase “differences that make a difference” to describe an emic approach, which I can illustrate with a simple example.
In Cambodia it is considered disrespectful, as a general rule, to show bare skin on shoulders or knees in public. Some men get away with going shirtless if the weather is hot and they are involved in heavy labor, but otherwise everyone is expected to cover their shoulders. This rule is rigidly enforced in sacred places, such as pagodas. People are denied entry if their bare shoulders are visible. But, covering one’s shoulders is not straightforward. One cannot simply drape a scarf, shawl, or other loose piece of cloth over bare shoulders and expect to be admitted to a pagoda. In European terms, shoulders with skin visible are bare, but shoulders covered with a shawl are not bare. The emics of Khmer culture are different. For your shoulders to be considered “covered” you must be wearing a fitted garment that has sleeves, such as a shirt or jacket, so that the skin on your shoulders is not visible. That is, simple visibility is not the issue. How the skin on your shoulders is covered matters. In Khmer emics: shoulders with skin showing and shoulders covered by loose material = bare; shoulders covered with a fitted garment = not bare. In this case, bare skin versus shawl on skin is a difference that does not make a difference. That is, it is an etic difference, not an emic one, to Khmer people.
Malinowski valued probing Trobriand Islanders’ emic view of the world, but he was also interested in an etic approach. He writes:
Not even the most intelligent native has any clear idea of the Kula as a big, organised social construction, still less of its sociological function and implications … The integration of all the details observed, the achievement of a sociological synthesis of all the various, relevant symptoms, is the task of the Ethnographer … the Ethnographer has to construct the picture of the big institution, very much as the physicist constructs his theory from the experimental data, which always have been within reach of everybody, but needed a consistent interpretation.
(Malinowski 1961 [1922]: 83–84)
Malinowski’s early history as a physicist is clear here, and his comments raise questions about fieldwork methods and their ultimate purpose. Here he is saying that fieldwork data are akin to experimental data in physical science in that in both cases the observer gathers the welter of observations together and is able to abstract overarching principles from all the myriad details. This is a point of view that we must ponder very carefully. Is it the goal of ethnographer as social scientist to take a mass of field observations and reduce them down to social laws in the way that Isaac Newton took a mass of experimental observations and reduced them to the laws of motion, or is there a different goal in anthropology? This debate has swung back and forth for some time with no clear answers, although at present the overwhelming majority of cultural anthropologists reject the scientific model (see Part II).
Post-Colonial Anthropology
The step from hotel verandah to village hut was a major leap forward for ethnographic fieldwork, but it contained some baggage that was rarely acknowledged. No matter how much Malinowski wanted to be a “participant” in local activities, he was always going to be perceived as an outsider, and, as importantly, he was going to be categorized as a member of the colonial elite. This status cannot avoid coloring the relationship between ethnographer and people being recorded. Whether in interviews, participant-observation, or both, there always exists a power dynamic informed by the status of the parties involved.
It is not an accident that all prewar ethnographic fieldwork conducted by British social anthropologists took place among peoples who had been colonized and become part of the British empire. Nor is it a coincidence that many of their works, such as African Political Systems, edited by Meyer СКАЧАТЬ