The Behavior of Animals. Группа авторов
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Behavior of Animals - Группа авторов страница 34

Название: The Behavior of Animals

Автор: Группа авторов

Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited

Жанр: Биология

Серия:

isbn: 9781119109525

isbn:

СКАЧАТЬ when he uses the term instinct, I would replace it with the term “behavior system.”

      In speaking of the instincts it has been impossible to keep them separate from the emotional excitements which go with them. Objects of rage, love, fear, etc., not only prompt a man to outward deeds, but provoke characteristic alterations in his attitude and visage, and affect his breathing, circulation, and other organic functions in specific ways. When the outward deeds are inhibited, these latter emotional expressions still remain, and we read the anger in the face, though the blow may not be struck, and the fear betrays itself in voice and color, though one may suppress all other sign. Instinctive reactions and emotional expressions thus shade imperceptibly into each other. Every object that excites an instinct excites an emotion as well. Emotions, however, fall short of instincts, in that the emotional reaction usually terminates in the subject’s own body, whilst the instinctive reaction is apt to go farther and enter into practical relations with the exciting object.(p. 442)

      He goes on to state his famous theory of emotion.

      Our natural way of thinking about these coarser emotions [grief, fear, rage, love] is that the mental perception of some fact excites the mental affection called the emotion, and that this latter state of mind gives rise to the bodily expression. My theory, on the contrary, is that the bodily changes follow directly the perception of the exciting fact, and that our feeling of the same changes as they occur is the emotion.(p. 449)

      In other words, when we meet a bear and run away, we are not running away because we are frightened; we are frightened because we are running away—fright is our perception of all the bodily changes that occur when we run away. This theory has been subject to much criticism to which I will return shortly. But first I will quote some more of James’ views.

      I have quoted from James so extensively because the ideas he expressed have continued to dominate studies of emotion from his time to the present. Historically, the idea that has been most discussed is his idea that emotion is the perception of the feedback one gets from bodily changes in response to some arousing situation. Everyone agrees that most emotional situations cause a multitude of visceral changes such as increases in heart rate, vasoconstriction, sweating, etc., all of which are caused by sympathetic nervous system action. However, Cannon (1927) challenged the notion that perception of these changes is the emotion for several reasons. He pointed out that the viscera are relatively insensitive structures and that visceral changes are too slow to account for emotional feelings that occur demonstrably quicker. He also cited results of Marañon, who injected epinephrine, a sympathetic nervous system stimulant, into human subjects and asked them to describe their feelings. The results showed a clear distinction “between the perception of the peripheral phenomena of vegetative emotion (i.e., the bodily changes) and the psychical emotion proper, which does not exist and which permits the subjects to report on the vegetative syndrome with serenity, without true feeling” (tr. by Cannon 1927, p. 113).

      Cannon also pointed out that the same visceral changes occur in very different emotional states as well as in nonemotional states. What distinguishes the different emotional states from each other? What is responsible for the “feeling” of each emotional state? James was aware of this problem, as we have seen, because he showed that there is no typical expression of each emotion. But James does list four “coarser” emotions and discusses a large number of “subtler” emotions, so presumably he thought that we can introspectively distinguish among them, perhaps on the basis of the nonorganic emotional expressions such as facial features or postures. Schachter and Singer (1962) proposed that the quality of an emotion is arrived at by a process of cognitive appraisal. In one of their experiments they injected epinephrine into human subjects. Some of the subjects were told what effects they might expect and others were not told anything about the effects. All subjects were then observed in the presence of a confederate of the experimenter who acted in either an elated or angry manner. The subjects were later asked about their emotional reactions. The informed subjects reported very little emotion (as also the subjects of Marañon); but the uninformed subjects did report emotional feelings, and the kind of emotion they felt tended to mimic that of the confederate. In other words, in precisely the same state of physiological arousal, emotional labels depended on the cognitive aspects of the situation.

      The appraisal approach assumes “that emotions are not merely triggered by objects in a reflexive or habitual way, but arise from a meaningful interpretation of an object by an individual” (p. 317). It considers the identification of emotional quality (its meaning) to require appraisal of the object and situation; it is the meaning that then leads to internal state changes: we are afraid of the bear when we see it (we appraise the situation) and then we become aroused and flee. Although Schachter & Singer use the word “appraisal” in their theory, Gendron & Barrett consider their theory closer in content to the psychological construction approach.

      The psychological construction approach posits that emotions are constructed out of more basic psychological ingredients that are not themselves specific to emotion. Two such basic components were proposed by Russell (2003): core affect and affective quality. “Core affect is that neurophysiological state consciously accessible as the simplest raw (nonreflective) feelings evident in moods and emotions” (p. 148). It is a single integral blend of two dimensions: pleasure-displeasure (which can range from elation to agony) and activation-deactivation (which can range from frenetic excitement to sleep). The feeling is an assessment of one’s current condition. Affective quality is a property of the stimulus: its capacity to change core affect. Perception of affective quality together with core affect allows a person to construct the emotion.

      Barrett (2013) believes that psychological construction constitutes a paradigm for the scientific study of emotion that is different from the “faculty” psychology paradigm of the basic emotion and appraisal approaches. She points to three principles of psychological construction that define this difference: the principles of variation, of core systems, and of emergentism and holism. I will not discuss these principles here, but I will say that in many respects the details of these principles bear a striking resemblance to many of the ideas originally СКАЧАТЬ