The Behavior of Animals. Группа авторов
Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Behavior of Animals - Группа авторов страница 11

Название: The Behavior of Animals

Автор: Группа авторов

Издательство: John Wiley & Sons Limited

Жанр: Биология

Серия:

isbn: 9781119109525

isbn:

СКАЧАТЬ imply that analysis is unnecessary because the whole is always more than the sum of its parts. They also rejected the focus of most learning theorists on the input/output relations of the whole organism, with neglect of the “physiological machinery,” and the sterility of the artificial environments used to study animals in many psychological laboratories.

      The term “ethology” has been applied primarily to the work of students who, though differing widely in the problems they tackled, the methods they used, the level of analysis at which they worked, and the theoretical interpretations (if any) that they adopted, shared certain orienting attitudes. They insisted that the proper description of behavior is a necessary preliminary to its analysis; and that the behavior of an animal must be studied in relation to the environment to which it has become adapted in evolution. In addition they held that full understanding of behavior required knowledge not only of its development and causation but also of its biological function and its evolution. The result was a vast amount of data on the behavior of animals and a certain amount of model-building to elucidate the mechanisms underlying behavior. In 1973 Lorenz and Tinbergen (together with von Frisch) were awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology and Medicine.

      It seems to happen not infrequently in the history of science that those regarded as originating a branch of science are subsequently seen to have been wrong in many of their generalizations. For instance, Freud (psychoanalysis) used a misleading model of motivation, Piaget (developmental psychology) based generalizations on a tiny sample of subjects, and Jeffreys (geophysics) refused to accept the evidence for continental drift. This was also the case with ethology. Many of the concepts that had been invaluable tools in the early days of ethology—the “innate releasing mechanism” and “fixed action pattern” for instance—were subsequently seen to involve oversimplification, and now seldom figure in the literature. But not surprisingly the change in outlook was not adopted simultaneously by all ethologists, and this led to some divisions within ethology. Lorenz, whose influence was particularly strong in Germany and the USA (through two research workers who had worked with him, Hess and Barlow), was slower to relinquish the innate/acquired dichotomy and energy model of motivation than Tinbergen and workers in the Netherlands and the UK.

      An issue important for the nature/nurture debate became prominent in the 1960s. Both Tinbergen and Lorenz had long argued on the basis of empirical evidence that species were specially equipped for particular learning tasks that were biologically important for them. Thorpe’s book on birdsong, published in 1961, showed that the chaffinch was predisposed to learn the species-characteristic song pattern. A few years later, Rozin, Garcia, and others demonstrated a predisposition to avoid toxins in mammals. Such findings were directly contrary to the orientation of the learning theorists, who were searching for laws of learning valid for all species and all situations. It thus became apparent that, in many cases, what was “innate” was a predisposition to learn some things in particular contexts. This was to be of special importance for the study of human behavior.

      At the same time, the influence of ethology started to penetrate into a number of other disciplines. Lehrman and Rosenblatt, as well as Beach and his many students, adopted the orienting attitudes of ethology in their work on behavioral endocrinology. Von Holst had already studied the elicitation of fixed action patterns by brain stimulation through implanted electrodes, and the importance of using unconfined animals where possible was recognized by neurophysiologists. Bowlby, a psychoanalyst concerned with the effects of maternal deprivation in children, realized that what had been called the “irrational fears of childhood” (fear of falling, being alone, etc.) would have been highly adaptive in the environments in which early hominids lived, and an ethological element was incorporated in the “attachment theory” which he elaborated, an approach that was to become central in studies of child development. The study of human nonverbal communication profited from the input of ethologists, such as Eibl Eibesfeldt. The techniques of the behavioral ecologists were applied in studies of preindustrial human groups. An ethological influence is to be seen in studies of human personal relationships, and even in studies of religion and morality. Thus, while ethology as a set of concepts or as a theory of animal behavior has been largely superseded, the influence of its orienting attitudes has increased and is potent in other disciplines.

      While behavioral ecology took center stage in the study of animal behavior, many felt it to be impoverished by the neglect of problems of development and causation. This book will go a long way toward setting the balance straight. Each of the four problems is covered, and the chapters introduce the growing points in the study of animal behavior at the start of the twenty-first century.

      The idea for this book arose out of a need that we (and many of our colleagues) felt for a comprehensive textbook on animal behavior. There is no shortage of animal behavior textbooks, so why did we want to produce a new one? First, animal behavior is a dynamic field of research, and we believe that a modern textbook should incorporate all the contemporary subdisciplines of behavioral biology, such as animal welfare, evolutionary psychology, animal cognition, and behavioral neuroscience. In some ways, the science СКАЧАТЬ